Neural nets and automata

print Print
Please select which sections you would like to print:
verifiedCite
While every effort has been made to follow citation style rules, there may be some discrepancies. Please refer to the appropriate style manual or other sources if you have any questions.
Select Citation Style
Feedback
Corrections? Updates? Omissions? Let us know if you have suggestions to improve this article (requires login).
Thank you for your feedback

Our editors will review what you’ve submitted and determine whether to revise the article.

The finite automata of McCulloch and Pitts

Part of automata theory lying within the area of pure mathematical study is often based on a model of a portion of the nervous system in a living creature and on how that system with its complex of neurons, nerve endings, and synapses (separating gap between neurons) can generate, codify, store, and use information. The “all or none” nature of the threshold of neurons is often referred to in formulating purely logical schemata or in constructing the practical electronic gates of computers. Any physical neuron can be sufficiently excited by an oncoming impulse to fire another impulse into the network of which it forms a part, or else the threshold will not be reached because the stimulus is absent or inadequate. In the latter case, the neuron fails to fire and remains quiescent. When several neurons are connected together, an impulse travelling in a particular part of the network may have several effects. It can inhibit another neuron’s ability to release an impulse; it can combine with several other incoming impulses each of which is incapable of exciting a neuron to fire but that, in combination, may provide the threshold stimulus; or the impulse might be confined within a section of the nerve net and travel in a closed loop, in what is called “feedback.” Mathematical reasoning about how nerve nets work has been applied to the problem of how feedback in a computing machine can result in an essential ingredient in the calculational process.

Original work on this aspect of automata theory was done by Warren S. McCulloch and Walter Pitts at the Research Laboratory of Electronics at the Massachusetts Institute of Technology starting in the 1940s.

The definitions of various automata as used here are based on the work of two mathematicians, John von Neumann and Stephen Cole Kleene, and the earlier neurophysiological researches of McCulloch and Pitts, which offer a mathematical description of some essential features of a living organism. The neurological model is suggested from studies of the sensory receptor organs, internal neural structure, and effector organs of animals. Certain responses of an animal to stimuli are known by controlled observation, and, since the pioneering work of a Spanish histologist, Santiago Ramón y Cajal, in the latter part of the 19th and early part of the 20th century, many neural structures have been well known. For the purposes of this article, the mathematical description of neural structure, following the neurophysiological description, will be called a “neural net.” The net alone and its response to input data are describable in purely mathematical terms.

A neural net may be conveniently described in terms of the kind of geometric configuration that suggests the physical structure of a portion of the brain. The component parts in the geometric form of a neural net are named (after the physically observed structures) neurons. Diagrammatically they could be represented by a circle and a line (together representing the body, or soma, of a physiological neuron) leading to an arrowhead or a solid dot (suggesting an endbulb of a neuron). A neuron may be assumed to have either an excitatory or an inhibitory effect on a succeeding one; and it may possess a threshold, or minimum number of unit messages, so to speak, that must be received from other neurons before it can be activated to fire an impulse. The process of transmission of excitation mimics that which is observed to occur in the nervous system of an animal. Messages of unit excitation are transmitted from one neuron to the next, and excitation is passed along the neural net in quantized form, a neuron either becoming excited or remaining non-excited, depending on the states (excitatory or quiescent) of neurons whose endbulbs impinge upon it. Specifically, neuron N, with threshold h, will be excited at time t, if and only if h or more neurons whose excitatory endbulbs impinge upon it are excited at time t - 1 and no neuron whose inhibitory endbulb impinges upon it is excited at time t - 1. A consistent picture can be made of these conditions only if time and excitation are quantized (or pulsed). It is assumed conventionally that a unit of time is required for the transmission of a message by any neuron.

Certain neurons in the configuration mathematically represent the physiological receptors that are excited or left quiescent by the exterior environment. These are called input neurons. Other neurons called output neurons record the logical value, excited or quiescent, of the whole configuration after time delay t and transmit an effect to an exterior environment. All the rest stimulate inner neurons.

Any geometric or logical description of the neural structure of an organism formulated as the basis of physical construction must be sufficiently simple to permit mechanical, electric, or electronic simulation of the neurons and their interconnections.

The basic logical organs

The types of events that can excite the automaton and the kinds of responses that it can make must next be considered. By stripping the description down to the most simple cases, the basic organs from which more complicated robots can be constructed may be discovered. Three basic organs (or elementary automata) are necessary, each corresponding to one of the three logical operations of language: the binary operations of disjunction and conjunction, leading to such propositions as AB (read “A or B”), AB (read “A and B”), and the unary operation of negation or complementation, leading to such propositions as Ac (read “not A” or “complement of A”). First to be considered are the stimulus-response pattern of these elementary automata.

Assuming that a neuron can be in only one of two possible states—i.e., excited or quiescent—an input neuron at a given instant of time t - 1 must be either excited or nonexcited by its environment. An environmental message transmitted to two input neurons N1 and N2 at time t - 1 can then be represented numerically in any one of the four following ways, in which binary digit 1 represents excitation and binary digit 0 represents quiescence: (0, 0), (0, 1), (1, 0), (1, 1). The disjunction automaton must be such that a single output neuron M correspondingly registers at time t the response: 0, 1, 1, 1. The conjunction automaton must be such that a single output neuron M correspondingly registers at time t the response: 0, 0, 0, 1. The negation automaton considered as having two input neurons N1 and N2, of which N1 is always excited, must respond to the environmental messages (1, 0) and (1, 1) with 1, 0, respectively, at the output neuron M.