- Key People:
- Georg von Békésy
- Related Topics:
- human ear
- inner ear
- bone conduction
- hearing
- air conduction
The tympanal organ of insects consists of a group of scolophores associated with a thin, horny (chitinous) membrane at the surface of the body, one on each side. Usually the scolophores are attached at one end by a spinous process to the tympanic membrane (eardrum); the other ends rest on an immobile part of the body structure. When the membrane moves back and forth in response to the alternating pressures of sound waves, the nerve fibre from the ganglion cell of the scolophore transmits impulses to the central nervous system. Because the tympanic membrane is activated by the pressure of sound waves, this is a pressure type of ear.
Simple tympanal organs, such as those found in moths, contain only two or four elements, or scolophores. In cicadas, on the other hand, these organs are highly developed; they include a sensory body (a number of scolophores in a capsule) that may contain as many as 1,500 elements.
With 80 to 100 scolophores, the grasshopper ear, which has been studied more thoroughly than any other insect ear, is structurally between that of moths and cicadas. Ordinarily, the tympanic membrane is hidden beneath the base of the insect’s wing cover. A bundle of auditory nerve fibres runs from one side of the sensory body, which lies on the inner surface of the membrane, and joins other nerve fibres of the region to form a large nerve extending to a ganglion (nerve centre) in the thorax.
Evidence of hearing and communication in insects
Behavioral observations
That the insect ear serves an auditory purpose has been proved by a large number of experimental observations, particularly those that have dealt most extensively with katydids and crickets. Males of these groups produce sounds by stridulation, which usually involves rubbing the covers of the wings together in a particular way. One wing has a serrated surface (a “file”) that runs along an enlarged vein; the other wing has a sharp edge over which the file is scraped. The scraping causes the wing surfaces to vibrate; the natural resonances of the vibrations and the particular rhythm and repetition rate of the scraping movements determine the nature of the song, which varies with each species. Most females are silent, but those of a few species have a poorly developed stridulatory apparatus, and weak sounds have been reported. Both males and females have tympanal organs for sound reception.
The observation that the males of many insect species produce repeated stridulatory sounds during the mating season led to the inference that the primary purpose of these noises was to attract a female. That this is indeed the case was first established by the extensive observations of the Yugoslavian entomologist Ivan Regen, who worked over the period 1902–30 mostly with a few species of katydids and crickets. In one of his earliest experiments, Regen proved (1913–14) that a male katydid of the species Thamnotrizon apterus responds to the sound of another male by chirping. The first male responds in turn to the second male’s chirp, and the two insects then set up an alternating pattern of chirping. Although this pattern had been observed earlier, Regen was the first to prove by a series of experiments that it depends upon the sense of hearing. After removing the forelegs, on which the tympanal organs are located, of certain males, he found that even though these insects continued to stridulate, they did so only in individual rhythms that were not affected by the sounds of other males. Any alternation of chirping between deafened males, or between a deafened and a normal male, occurred only rarely, for brief times, and by chance.
A long series of check experiments by Regen showed that other stimuli, such as light, odours, and surface vibrations, did not affect the chirping behaviour. In these experiments the insects were placed in separate rooms, and their sounds were transmitted by telephone.
Further experiments carried out by Regen on field crickets (Liogryllus campestris) demonstrated the reactions of females to chirping males. In the most elaborate of these experiments, 1,600 sexually receptive females were released around the periphery of a large enclosed area in the middle of which had been placed a cage containing one or more chirping males. Precise data concerning the frequency with which the females moved toward the cage were obtained by surrounding the cage site with an array of traps in which the females were caught as they moved inward. The results were statistically significant. Normal females (those with intact tympanal organs) moved toward the cage and eventually reached it. The removal of one foreleg and its tympanal organ, however, caused difficulty; the movements were more random and the approaches fewer, although some females did succeed in reaching the cage. When both tympanal organs were removed or if the male failed to chirp, the performance of the females was reduced to chance. They also failed to exhibit the seeking performances if the male’s stridulatory organ was modified, as by removing the file, so that little or no sound was produced.
In 1926 Regen returned to his study of the alternating chirping pattern of katydids and succeeded in having males react to an artificial sound, one that Regen himself produced. He also found that the alternation could be demonstrated with a suitably active male by using a variety of sounds—whistles, percussion noises, and sounds made with his mouth. It was never altogether clear, however, what changes Regen had made in his signals that finally brought success; probably the secret lay in the particular rhythm and timing of the signals. At any rate, this method made possible a study of the general nature of the auditory sensitivity of these insects and the range of sound frequencies to which they responded. It was shown that katydids are most sensitive to the very high frequencies, those that are beyond the limit of the human ear. The instruments available to Regen at the time, however, did not permit a precise measurement of intensity thresholds. (A threshold is the lowest point at which a particular stimulus will cause a response in an organism.)
Although the work of Regen and others established the basic character of sound reception in insects and its role in communication and mating, other details had to await the introduction of electrophysiological methods in this field as well as the development of electronic methods for the precise production, control, and measurement of sound stimuli.
Electrophysiological observations
When making electrophysiological observations of an auditory mechanism, an electrode (one terminal, generally a fine wire, in an electric circuit) is placed on a nerve or some other sensory structure in the mechanism. Sounds, presented at different frequencies and intensities, produce neural or sensory changes, which are actually electrical discharges or changes in electrical potential of extremely small magnitude. The impulses are picked up by the electrode and transmitted to an instrument with which they can be amplified, observed, and recorded. In both behavioral and electrophysiological observations, the auditory sensitivity of an animal to sounds of different frequencies can be illustrated by a curve.
The electrophysiological method was first used in research on the insect ear in 1933, with observations mainly on two katydid and one cricket species. The tympanal organ of these insects is located on one of the segments of the foreleg; its nerve goes to a ganglion in the thorax. When an electrode is placed on this nerve, its threshold sensitivity and overall frequency range can be determined by varying the intensity and frequency of the sounds applied to the tympanic membrane. It has been found that the tympanal organ of these insects responds poorly to low tones (those of low frequency) but improves rapidly as the frequency increases to a maximum sensitivity around 3,000 to 5,000 hertz. For higher frequencies the sensitivity declines, until a limit is reached at 30,000 hertz. It is likely that the insect’s identification of its own species by means of song is primarily in terms of intensity and time patterns, with the rapid changes of intensity playing a prominent part. The possibility of frequency also entering into the pattern, however, cannot be ruled out.
A further question concerns the perception of the direction of a sound source. Clearly, if a female is to seek out and find a chirping male, the effectiveness of her performance depends upon an ability to localize the sound. Experiments indicate that the magnitude of electric responses from the tympanal nerve in katydids varies in a systematic manner when a given sound is presented at different angles while the distance is held constant. The insects continue to exhibit this directional pattern even after one of the tympanal organs has been removed. As was mentioned earlier, Regen found that female crickets deprived of one tympanal organ were still able to locate a chirping male, though less effectively than when both organs were intact.
Evidence of hearing and communication in spiders
Whether spiders have a sense of hearing has long been debated. Early anecdotal observations concerning this matter have now been reinforced with both behavioral and electrophysiological evidence showing without doubt that spiders are sensitive to mechanical vibrations and also to aerial sounds. Whether this sensitivity should be regarded as hearing is considered later in this section, after a review of the anatomical and behavioral evidence.
Anatomical evidence
The bodies of spiders contain many slitlike openings, called lyriform organs, that have been considered as sensory in nature. Most of these organs probably have a kinesthetic function and thus provide information on local movements of body parts. There is one type of lyriform organ, however, that differs from the others in its location and in certain structural details. It is found on the metatarsal (next to last) segment of each of the eight legs, close to the joint that this segment makes with the tarsus (the last segment, or foot), and consists of a number of slits—about 10 in the common house spider—that partially encircle the leg. Each slit contains a fluid chamber the inner wall of which is pierced by a tubule through which a thin filament runs to one of the two side walls (lamellae) that enclose the slit. This filament is evidently the termination of a ganglion cell that lies deeper in the leg. It has been suggested that an alternating compression of the lamellae stimulates the terminal filament.

The responsiveness of the common house spider to aerial sounds and mechanical vibrations includes a wide range, from below 20 to as high as 45,000 hertz. Within this range the sensitivity, as measured by electrical potentials, varies widely for aerial sounds; in some experiments narrow regions of frequency have been found in which no responses could be obtained at the highest intensities available. These variations of sensitivity are ascribed to mechanical resonances in the lyriform structure.
The tarsus evidently plays an important part in responses to sounds. Removal of portions of the tarsus reduces the responses about in proportion to the amount removed; immobilization of the tarsus greatly impairs the sensitivity. It appears, therefore, that the tarsus serves as a sensing element that transmits vibrations to the lyriform organ, which thus is a velocity type of ear.
Behavioral evidence
It has been reported that spiders react in characteristic ways to a buzzing insect caught in their web. The spider apparently locates the insect at once, runs to it, and attacks it. An inactive object, however, such as a small pebble enmeshed in the web, produces a different response: the spider manipulates the strands of the web, locates the object, and cuts away the filaments surrounding it so that the object drops to the ground. The reactions of a house spider to a mechanical vibrator applied to a point on the web have been observed. Such a stimulus elicits a response similar to that of an active insect if the vibratory frequency is between 400 and 700 hertz. For frequencies above 1,000 hertz, however, the spider reacts either by running to a secluded corner of the web or, if the intensity is too great, by abandoning the web altogether. From this and similar evidence it has been concluded that the spider has the ability of pitch (tone) discrimination between low and high ranges and perhaps can distinguish between tones of the lower range.
Spiders also react to aerial tones from an artificial source, such as a loudspeaker. These stimuli elicit an orientation response, in which the spider faces the source and reaches out with the two front legs. Thus, in view of the high level of sensitivity to both aerial and mechanical stimuli, the reception of sounds in the spider can probably be regarded as true hearing, and the lyriform organ as a form of ear. It is evidently a velocity type of ear, for there is no tympanic surface to respond to sound pressures, and the small leg segments seem to respond to the oscillatory motions of the air particles.